approve disapprove neither
22 50 28
POLSCI 240 / PSY 225: Political Psychology
January 27, 2025
What is the nature of public opinion?
“…a belief system [is a] configuration of ideas and attitudes in which the elements are bound together by some form of constraint or functional interdependence.”
“In the static case, ‘constraint’ [means] the success we would have in predicting, given initial knowledge that an individual holds a specified attitude, that he hold certain further ideas and attitudes.”
“In the dynamic case, ‘constraint’ [means] the probability that a change in the perceived status (truth, desirability, and so forth) of one idea-element would psychologically require, from the point of view of the actor, some compensating change(s) in the status of idea-elements elsewhere in the configuration.”
Converse (1964), “The Nature of Belief Systems in Mass Publics”
What are some reasons why we might think it desirable that citizens have structured (constrained) beliefs
Derived from 1st principles
Similarity to, and understanding of, “elite” ideology (e.g., liberal vs conservative)
Beliefs fit together, are logically consistent
Why should people hold opinions that look like partisan elites?
If change is systematic, opinions more correlated when closer in time
What kind of model could explain the weird pattern in Converse’s data?
A small percent of public with perfectly stable attitudes
A much larger percent with random responses to survey questions (“non-attitudes”)
Much of political psychology in the subsequent years is a response, in one way or another, to these conclusions
Classic measurement theory says that a measure of something is the sum of the “true score” plus “random error”
\[ \begin{align} \text{measured score} &= \text{true score} + \text{error} \\ x &= \tau + e \end{align} \]
Let’s say you are 6ft tall, and measurements of your height are typically accurate to about 1/8 of an inch:
Height is not changing (perfectly stable), but measured height changes randomly across time
If same is true of political opinions, measurement error can account for the pattern found by Converse!
Thus, an alternative view of Converse’s data: people have roughly stable opinions, but these are measured with lots of error
We have empirical facts about democratic publics (true for the most part, even in contemporary politics)
We have two different models (theories) that can account for these facts
Converse’s “black and white” model (most have “non-attitudes”)
Measurement error: hard to translate (stable) opinions into survey responses, so lots of random error
Take a few seconds and answer the following questions - keep answers to yourself, but really try, as if you were taking a survey
Do you approve, disapprove, or neither approve nor disapprove of the job Joe Biden did as President?
Do you think tariffs on imported goods from China to the United States should be higher than they are now, lower, or should they stay the same?
Simple recall
Constructionist models
Imagine you are asked whether you approve or disapprove of President Trump, one time each day, for 100 days
approve disapprove neither
22 50 28
We might expect that most people are ambivalent when it comes to most political issues and objects
They have, stored in long-term memory, both “positive” and “negative” considerations relevant to evaluating an object
When they construct their opinion, they get a random sample
So their opinions are likely to be:
When you think about whether you approve or disapprove of Trump, what comes to mind and why?
What’s going on here more generally?
Accessibility is how likely it is that something in long-term memory gets brought into working memory
And do people just take in whatever considerations they come across?
Receive, Accept, Sample
Partisan political elites generate messages and disseminate them to public through media
Politically attentive people are more likely to receive political messages
Politically attentive people are less likely to accept messages that conflict with their political values and identities (e.g., partisanship)
Political opinions are the average of sampled considerations from long-term memory
Public opinion is highly responsive to “context”
Agenda-setting: the issues that get coverage have disproportionate influence on public opinion
Priming: potentially irrelevant aspects of political coverage can alter public opinion through spreading activation
Framing: highlighting certain aspects of issues over others can change public opinion
Partisan polarization: public opinion divides by partisanship when elite partisans are divided, and polarization is larger for more attentive citizens
Iyengar and Simon (1993)
Nelson et al. (1997)
Berinsky (2007)